About: http://data.cimple.eu/claim-review/c810406adc8d43be0ac13f1f0ac5092e63dc85eef895e01f1df1e2ab     Goto   Sponge   NotDistinct   Permalink

An Entity of Type : schema:ClaimReview, within Data Space : data.cimple.eu associated with source document(s)

AttributesValues
rdf:type
http://data.cimple...lizedReviewRating
schema:url
schema:text
  • Many Americans are unaware that there are two correct versions of a lowercase "G" — and even fewer people are able to correctly identify and write each version if they're asked, according to research conducted by Johns Hopkins University researchers. The findings could hold implications for how future generations learn to read and write in a mostly digital world. Overall, just one-quarter of study participants were able to properly recognize the most commonly used English version of the lowercase print "G." The Different Types of Gs When it comes to print, the letter "G" has two lowercase versions: the “opentail,” which looks like a fishhook and is commonly used in handwriting, and the “looptail,” which is often used in printed or typed material. Both the opentail and looptail versions of "G" are what are known as “allographs,” or different, yet distinct, letter shapes that correspond to the same letter identity. Although the looptail is most commonly used in printed materials, most people never learn how to write or properly identify this version. (There are other allographs of the letter "G," including uppercase printed "G" and upper- and lower-case cursive "G.") “We think that if we look at something enough, especially if we have to pay attention to its shape as we do during reading, then we would know what it looks like, but our results suggest that’s not always the case,” said Michael McCloskey, a cognitive scientist at Johns Hopkins Krieger School of Arts and Sciences, in a news release at the time. “What we think may be happening here is that we learn the shapes of most letters in part because we have to write them in school. Looptail 'G' is something we’re never taught to write, so we may not learn its shape as well.” What the Study Entailed As part of their experiment, the researchers first surveyed fonts used in books and found that the looptail appeared in 74% of children’s picture books, 89% of children’s chapter books, and 97% of books for adults. To test how well a person might be able to identify either version, they then turned to a three-part experiment that has been published in a 2018 edition of the Journal of Experimental Psychology. In the first segment, 38 participants — an admittedly small sample size — were asked whether they knew that there were two forms of a lowercase "G." Only two participants were able to recall the looptail version, and only one person was able to write it correctly. As part of the second experiment, 16 participants — an even smaller test pool — were asked to read passages from a book and say any word containing a "G" aloud. They were then asked to write the form of "G" that they had seen. Half of those participants wrote an opentail "G" and only one person wrote the looptail accurately. For the final test, study participants were asked to correctly identify a looptail "G" in a multiple-choice test. Just seven of 25 participants were able to do so. What the Findings Mean The researchers concluded that these “surprising” gaps in readers’ knowledge hold implications for understanding how we interpret letters and how that knowledge is acquired. It also implies that a person’s knowledge of a letter may decline when they are not being regularly written out. “What about children who are just learning to read? Do they have a little bit more trouble with this form of 'g' because they haven’t been forced to pay attention to it and write it?” said McCloskey. “That’s something we don’t really know. Our findings give us an intriguing way of looking at questions about the importance of writing for reading. Here is a naturally occurring situation where unlike most letters, this is a letter we don’t write. We could ask whether children have some reading disadvantage with this form of 'g.'” https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=iSNGaJ0KiEs&feature=emb_logo
schema:mentions
schema:reviewRating
schema:author
schema:datePublished
schema:inLanguage
  • English
schema:itemReviewed
Faceted Search & Find service v1.16.115 as of Oct 09 2023


Alternative Linked Data Documents: ODE     Content Formats:   [cxml] [csv]     RDF   [text] [turtle] [ld+json] [rdf+json] [rdf+xml]     ODATA   [atom+xml] [odata+json]     Microdata   [microdata+json] [html]    About   
This material is Open Knowledge   W3C Semantic Web Technology [RDF Data] Valid XHTML + RDFa
OpenLink Virtuoso version 07.20.3238 as of Jul 16 2024, on Linux (x86_64-pc-linux-musl), Single-Server Edition (126 GB total memory, 5 GB memory in use)
Data on this page belongs to its respective rights holders.
Virtuoso Faceted Browser Copyright © 2009-2025 OpenLink Software